google search

自訂搜尋

akk's blog

2009年3月10日 星期二

conceptual agnosia

http://crystaldream-panoramic-panoramic.blogspot.com/2007/10/asymptomatic-perception.html

Asymptomatic Perception

In The Aesthetic Ideology, Paul de Man reads a description of the sublime in Kant's Third Critique as a vision without synthesis--the sea as a floor, the sky as a ceiling, affectless, mere lines of the world as indifferent as those of a hotel room. The way de Man reads this passage sharply contrasts with the characteristic reading of Kant's sublime as a Romantic vision of imposing ravines, ravishing Nature, that conjures up an innate human desire for more knowledge. In his study of attention in modernity, Jonathan Crary writes, "Alongside the discovery of the linguistic disorders grouped under the category of aphasia, a set of related visual disruptions was described [by reseachers into linguistic and perceptual pathologies in the 1870s and 1880s] by the resonant term agnosia. Agnosia was one of the primary asymbolias, or impairments , of a hypothetical symbolic function. Essentially, it described a purely visual awareness to an object . . . an inability to make any conceptual or symbolic identification of an object, a failure of recognition, a condition in which visual information was experienced as a kind of primal strangeness" (56). Crary connects this research into agnosia with the increasing construction of perception in the latter half of the nineteenth-century as the work of the perceiving subject rather than the passive reception of the classical subject. Along with such perceptual agency, according to Crary, comes the idea of a reality socially contracted, of what Pierre Janet calls "the reality function" (56). If what de Man sees in Kant is a form of agnosia, could this be construed as a kind of "modernist sublime," a vision as yet unmoored from a social body which would place it? How might such a sublime connect back to the awareness and representation of the body as locus of perception? How might it connect to cinematic vision, which is purely synthesis of a lost referent?

Encephale. 2004 Nov-Dec;30(6):570-7. Links
[Awareness of deficits and anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease.][Article in French]


Antoine C, Antoine P, Guermonprez P, Frigard B.
Centre Hospitalier Intercommunal de Wasquehal, Le Molinel, 59290 Wasquehal.

This article reviews studies concerning unawareness of deficits in Alzheimer's disease. Unawareness of the deficits associated with dementia has frequently been reported in clinical descriptions of the later stages of the disease. Consistent with the literature, we shall use the expressions impaired awareness, unawareness of deficits, anosognosia, and lack of insight interchangeably. Anosognosia can be defined as an impaired ability to recognize the presence or appreciate the severity of deficits in sensory, perceptual, motor, affective, or cognitive functioning. Unawareness has been operationally defined in a variety of ways. Unawareness can be measured as the discrepancy between the patient's self-report and the report of a natural caregiver or the clinical rating of a health care professional. The reports generally concern with several domains, most often memory domain. Discrepancy between subjective ratings and neuropsychological performance during clinical assessment has also been used to measure anosognosia. Advantages, limits and equivalence of these different methods are discussed. The impact of family burden has to be considered as a systematic methodological bias if the natural caregiver is implicated in the assessment. The psychometric properties of the clinical assessment have also to be discussed. The psychological nature of the discrepancy between patient's self-report and cognitive performances has to be analyzed and the necessity of ecological protocols, longitudinal assessment is discussed. The major results concerning prevalence, nature of anosognosia and the associated disorders are analyzed. In particular, the notion of heterogeneity of anosognosia and the correlates with depression, severity of dementia and executive dysfunction are developed. Prevalence is largely function of methodological choices and conceptual definition of anosognosia. Three major researches are compared and the contrast between their results (prevalence from 23% to 75% in AD) is analyzed. Particularly, the hypotheses about anosognosia play a great role in the findings. At first time of research, anosognosia was considered as a general symptom and so, studies were centered on the unawareness related to only one cognitive function. But the 90's findings suggest that patients with AD have impaired awareness for some types of deficits (affective or cognitive functions) but can more accurately appraise other deficits. Currently anosognosia cannot be considered as a unitary entity. It may be that patients with AD are unaware of some types of deficits, but are aware of others, and that nature and intensity of their anosognosia may change during the course of the dementia. It has been proposed that depression is more common when disease is mild and awareness of deficits is retained, and that depression becomes less common when disease increases and awareness declines. Depression is conceived as a psychological reaction. However, the correlations between anosognosia level and depression scores reveal either weak relationships or no relationships. Alternative hypothesis is that anosognosia is related to overall dementia severity and to memory impairment. However, correlations of unawareness of deficits, i.e. the difference between self-report and relative's -rating, with a measure of dementia and with patient's performance on objective memory tasks did not reveal strong, consistent relationships between degree of anosognosia and severity of dementia or of memory impairment. On the other hand, the best neuropsychological predictors of impaired insight are Trail Making Test or Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, i.e. tests that have been shown to be sensitive to a frontal lobe dysfunction. SPECT measures of regional cerebral blood flow have been used in the study of anosognosia. The main findings are that unawareness in AD is associated with hypoperfusion of the right dorsolateral frontal lobe. Anosognosia may result from the disruption of broader cognitive process that is subsumed by the frontal lobes. The mechanisms of unawareness are not well known and studies are essentially descriptive works and try to give information about pre-valence or clinical associated disorders of anosognosia. Several authors have proposed that unawareness is part of a defensive mechanism that would protect demented patients from depressive feelings. Other authors have proposed that anosognosia may result from dysfunction in specific brain areas. It is suggested that anosognosia in AD may result from greater impairment of a central executive system, which is a metacognitive structure that is involved in planning, cognitive resource allocation, and set shifting. The main problem with those both major hypotheses is their incapacity to explain the heterogeneous impairment of awareness. Other authors speculate that the impaired insight of Alzheimer's disease has several components, psychological and neuropsychological. This view doesn't seem convincing and new components have to be taken in account in order to propose a theoretical framework about anosognosia in AD. Environmental and dispositional components and an interactional view could be interesting. Those possible directions for future research and solutions concerning methodological and conceptual problems are outlined. In particular, a neuro-psycho-social view of unawareness is introduced.

PMID: 15738860 [PubMed - indexed for MEDLINE]

1: Hum Brain Mapp. 2001 Aug;13(4):199-212.Links
A role for left temporal pole in the retrieval of words for unique entities.Grabowski TJ, Damasio H, Tranel D, Ponto LL, Hichwa RD, Damasio AR.
Department of Neurology, University of Iowa College of Medicine, Iowa City, Iowa 52242, USA. thomas-grabowski@uiowa.edu

Both lesion and functional imaging studies have implicated sectors of high-order association cortices of the left temporal lobe in the retrieval of words for objects belonging to varied conceptual categories. In particular, the cortices located in the left temporal pole have been associated with naming unique persons from faces. Because this neuroanatomical-behavioral association might be related to either the specificity of the task (retrieving a name at unique level) or to the possible preferential processing of faces by anterior temporal cortices, we performed a PET imaging experiment to test the hypothesis that the effect is related to the specificity of the word retrieval task. Normal subjects were asked to name at unique level entities from two conceptual categories: famous landmarks and famous faces. In support of the hypothesis, naming entities in both categories was associated with increases in activity in the left temporal pole. No main effect of category (faces vs. landmarks/buildings) or interaction of task and category was found in the left temporal pole. Retrieving names for unique persons and for names for unique landmarks activate the same brain region. These findings are consistent with the notion that activity in the left temporal pole is linked to the level of specificity of word retrieval rather than the conceptual class to which the stimulus belongs. Copyright 2001 Wiley-Liss, Inc.

PMID: 11410949 [PubMed - indexed for MEDLINE]
Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved.
Implicit integration in a case of integrative visual agnosia





References and further reading may be available for this article. To view references and further reading you must purchase this article.


Hillel Aviezera, Ayelet N. Landaub, c, Lynn C. Robertsonb, c, Mary A. Petersond, Nachum Sorokere, Yaron Sachere, Yoram Bonnehf and Shlomo Bentina, ,

aDepartment of Psychology, Hebrew University, 91905 Jerusalem, Israel

bUniversity of California, Berkeley, CA, United States

cVeterans Affairs Medical Research, Martinez, CA, United States

dUniversity of Arizona, AZ, United States

eLowenstein Rehabilitation Hospital, Raanana and Sackler Faculty of Medicine, Israel

fThe Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel


Received 24 August 2006; revised 29 January 2007; accepted 31 January 2007. Available online 9 February 2007.

Abstract
We present a case (SE) with integrative visual agnosia following ischemic stroke affecting the right dorsal and the left ventral pathways of the visual system. Despite his inability to identify global hierarchical letters [Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383], and his dense object agnosia, SE showed normal global-to-local interference when responding to local letters in Navon hierarchical stimuli and significant picture-word identity priming in a semantic decision task for words. Since priming was absent if these features were scrambled, it stands to reason that these effects were not due to priming by distinctive features. The contrast between priming effects induced by coherent and scrambled stimuli is consistent with implicit but not explicit integration of features into a unified whole. We went on to show that possible/impossible object decisions were facilitated by words in a word-picture priming task, suggesting that prompts could activate perceptually integrated images in a backward fashion. We conclude that the absence of SE's ability to identify visual objects except through tedious serial construction reflects a deficit in accessing an integrated visual representation through bottom-up visual processing alone. However, top-down generated images can help activate these visual representations through semantic links.

Keywords: Integrative agnosia; Visual agnosia; Local–global processing; TPJ; Grouping

鬼視/詭視

http://www.stc-access.org/2009/01/23/charles-bonnet-syndrome-phantom-vision/

Charles Bonnet Syndrome (phantom vision)

For an informative article about phantom vision, which affects between 10 and 40 percent of people with low vision, read the article on Lighthouse International, “I See Purple Flowers Everywhere: The Many Visions of Charles Bonnet Syndrome” by Lylas G.Mogk,MD, and Marja Mogk, PhD; with Carol J. Sussman-Skalka,CSW, MBA.

Do you ever see things you know are not there but look real anyway? It’s a common side effect among people with vision impairment. While we refer to it as “phantom vision,” the technical term is “Charles Bonnet Syndrome.” If you’ve experienced this, rest easy. Your mind is fine. It’s your eyes that are playing tricks on you.

What Exactly Is Charles Bonnet Syndrome?Charles Bonnet, an 18th century Swiss naturalist and philosopher, is credited as the first person to describe the syndrome. Like his grandfather, who had low vision and saw men, women, birds and buildings he knew were not there, Charles experienced similar phantom visions when his own vision deteriorated.

One explanation compares this condition to phantom limb experiences. People who have a limb amputated may still feel their toes or fingers, or may experience itching on an arm that is not there. This happens because the limb’s nerves are still active and sending signals to the brain, which the brain interprets as sensations from the missing limb. Similarly, when retinal cells become impaired and are no longer able to receive and relay visual images to the brain, the visual system begins firing off images on its own.

Often, these images are not related at all to a person’s life. Sam, who has macular degeneration, said, “I see little monkeys with red hats and blue coats playing in the front yard.” Sam had no doubt that the monkeys he saw were not real ones. As a result, he wasn’t concerned about his mind. However, he was worried about what others would think, so he kept it to himself…

Lighthouse.org—Home > About Us > Newsletters and Publications > Sharing Solutions > Fall 2004 > Purple Flowers

The article continues on to explain the percentage of people who may be affected by Charles Bonnet Syndrome and that it is not a psychiatric problem. The article describes some of the images seen that patients have reported.

A majority of people do not find their phantom vision disturbing, probably because the images they see are amusing, pleasing or entertaining…

This article is based on, and includes quotes from, a chapter in Macular Degeneration: The Complete Guide to Saving and Maximizing Your Sight, written by Lylas G. Mogk, MD, and Marja Mogk, PhD, published by The Ballantine Publishing Group (2003).

This article appeared in Sharing Solutions - Fall 2004 Edition

====

http://www.lighthouse.org/aboutus/newsletters/sharing-solutions/fall-2004/purple-flowers/

"I See Purple Flowers Everywhere: The Many Visions of Charles Bonnet Syndrome"
by Lylas G.Mogk,MD, and Marja Mogk, PhD; with Carol J. Sussman-Skalka,CSW, MBA.

Do you ever see things you know are not there but look real anyway? It's a common side effect among people with vision impairment. While we refer to it as "phantom vision," the technical term is "Charles Bonnet Syndrome." If you've experienced this, rest easy. Your mind is fine. It's your eyes that are playing tricks on you.

What Exactly Is Charles Bonnet Syndrome?

Charles Bonnet, an 18th century Swiss naturalist and philosopher, is credited as the first person to describe the syndrome. Like his grandfather, who had low vision and saw men, women, birds and buildings he knew were not there, Charles experienced similar phantom visions when his own vision deteriorated.

One explanation compares this condition to phantom limb experiences. People who have a limb amputated may still feel their toes or fingers, or may experience itching on an arm that is not there. This happens because the limb's nerves are still active and sending signals to the brain, which the brain interprets as sensations from the missing limb. Similarly, when retinal cells become impaired and are no longer able to receive and relay visual images to the brain, the visual system begins firing off images on its own.

Often, these images are not related at all to a person's life. Sam, who has macular degeneration, said, "I see little monkeys with red hats and blue coats playing in the front yard." Sam had no doubt that the monkeys he saw were not real ones. As a result, he wasn't concerned about his mind. However, he was worried about what others would think, so he kept it to himself.

Is the Syndrome Common?

Studies show that Charles Bonnet Syndrome affects between 10 and 40 percent of people with low vision. Our research suggests that it is more likely to appear if you have visual acuity between 20/120 and 20/400. If your vision falls within these parameters, your eyes still have a great deal of power, even though they aren't receiving or sending as many images as previously. As a result, your eyes may be providing additional images of their own.

We can't predict whether you will see images and, if you do, how frequently they will occur or how long they will last. You may never experience Charles Bonnet Syndrome, you may have it for only a few months, or you may have it for years. You may see images everyday, a few times a week or several times a month.

Are You Sure This Is Not a Psychiatric Problem?

Yes! Charles Bonnet Syndrome is no more than a side effect of vision loss. The six criteria for Charles Bonnet Syndrome (outlined by Naville in 1873 - and still applicable today) can help you determine whether or not you are experiencing phantom vision. Ask yourself whether the images you see have the following six characteristics:

They occur when you are fully conscious and wide awake, often during broad daylight.
They do not deceive you; you are aware that they are not real.
They occur in combination with normal perception. For example, you may see a sidewalk clearly but find it covered with dots, flowers, or faces.
They are exclusively visual and do not appear in combination with any sounds or bizarre sensations.
They appear and disappear without obvious cause.
They are amusing or annoying but not grotesque.
"Since ophthalmology has paid so little attention to Charles Bonnet Syndrome, many doctors don't realize how common it really is, and some may not be familiar with it at all."

What Do People Actually See?

Patients "have reported seeing cartoon characters, flowers in the bathroom sink, hands rubbing each other, waterfalls and mountains, tigers, maple trees in vibrant autumn foliage, yellow polka dots, row houses, a dinner party and brightly colored balloons. Many people see faces or life-size figures that they've never seen before. One of the most remarkable qualities of these figures is that they almost always wear pleasant expressions and often make eye contact with the viewer. Menacing behavior, grotesque shapes and scenes of violent conflict are not, to my knowledge, a part of the syndrome.

"Usually the same image or set of images reappears to each person, sometimes in the same places or at the same time of day. Sam's monkeys usually materialized around sunset, cavorting across the lawn or around the big blue easy chair by the fireplace. They stayed for ten or 20 minutes several times a week for two years and then began to appear less frequently. ... Sometimes the images are exactly to scale and sometimes larger or smaller than life. ... Joe's horses looked like children's toys at a distance of ten feet, but as they galloped outside they became Clydesdales and then stallions worthy of Gulliver."

Rosa saw "flowering trees" with large, beautiful pink flowers. She knew that they weren't real because it was autumn. But when she told a friend what she was seeing, her friend was not convinced with Rosa's explanation. Rosa stopped telling people about her visions. She acknowledged, "You have to be careful what you say."

"Sometimes Charles Bonnet Syndrome images can become confused with dream images. For example, several of my patients have reported frightening moments when they thought they saw a man standing in their bedroom or hallway. These men, however, were often darkclad or indistinct figures that appeared as the patient was relaxing on a couch, dozing or in bed waiting to fall asleep, or just awaking. These figures were probably residual dream images. ... They are not typical of Charles Bonnet Syndrome."

Is Charles Bonnet Syndrome Treatable?

A majority of people do not find their phantom vision disturbing, probably because the images they see are amusing, pleasing or entertaining. You may want to discuss your phantom vision with your doctor, particularly if you feel bothered or distressed by your experiences. While there does not seem to be any specific medications that work for everyone, many people find that learning more about the condition helps to reassure them. Buddy's "purple flowers" appeared in his bathroom and on his clothing. He got a chuckle when he imagined what golf pants with purple flowers would look like to other people. He had a cousin with macular degeneration who saw frogs in his bathtub and a very attractive woman stationed on his back porch. Buddy's purple flowers didn't compare to the woman on Harry's porch; and Harry, though disappointed that his "vision" was not real, found that he enjoyed it quite a lot!


--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

This article is based on, and includes quotes from, a chapter in Macular Degeneration: The Complete Guide to Saving and Maximizing Your Sight, written by Lylas G. Mogk, MD, and Marja Mogk, PhD, published by The Ballantine Publishing Group (2003).


--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

This article appeared in Sharing Solutions - Fall 2004 Edition



2009年3月5日 星期四

方舟

女人無法從過去走出來
看不到前路 倒退到過去
喝著陳舊這老酒
回味甜辛酸苦種種
透過分析過去的相片
想起從前對憂鬱酒鬼自畫像的分析--

那麼的庸俗的洞悉
那麼的表淺的理解
難怪憂鬱酒鬼沒試過愛她
她確實不是知音

她想他之不快樂一定來自於挫折之壓抑
因為
他無法脫逃一些不愛的悲傷
無法弭平不愛的痛苦
他把自愛投射到認同之客體上
幻化成同性之愛 
一直不斷 愛上自己的化身
他又被娜西瑟司所附
無法愛上跟他長得不像的人

他看不起溫情 耽溺於冷冽的關係

他不愛清水 喜歡琴酒加馬丁尼
印在他心裏的不是血腥瑪麗 而是梅子酒
鑲嵌在舌苔下的紋理
酸澀混和著圓融的甜味包覆著的苦

suppose他也不愛黑咖啡
她和他 從過去到現在都沒有交集

女人吐了口煙 氤氳如霧瀰漫。

女人把自己囚禁在伊甸園
視而不見 虐肆天地的風雨
末日將至的預言
親吻著愛戀的亞當
自願被方舟軟禁

不情願踏出一步跨越橫亙的洪水
心裏的那一個
現實的那一個

2009年3月4日 星期三

標籤

追蹤者